Notes


PREFACE

1. Kathleen McAuliffe (2010), ‘If Modern Humans Are So Smart, Why Are Our Brains Shrinking?’, Discover magazine, September 2010. http://discovermagazine.com/2010/sep/25-modern-humans-smart-why-brain-shrinking#.UdGTQxxYdN0

2. D. H. Bailey and D. C. Geary (2009), ‘Hominid brain evolution: Testing climactic, ecological, and social competition models’, Human Nature, 20, 67–79.

3. D. C. T. Kruska (2005), ‘On the evolutionary significance of encephalization in some eutherian mammals: effects of adaptive radiation, domestication, and feralization’, Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 65, 73–108.

4. Claudio J. Bidau (2009), ‘Domestication through the centuries: Darwin’s ideas and Dmitry Belyaev’s long-term experiment in silver foxes’, Gayana 73 (Suplemento), 55–72.

5. Hilleke Pol et al. (2006), ‘Changing your sex changes your brain: influences of testosterone and estrogen on adult human brain structure’, European Journal of Endocrinology, 155, S107–S114.

6. K. Soproni, Á. Miklósi, J. Topál and V. Csányi (2001), ‘Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris)’, Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115, 122–6.

7. L. N. Trut (1999), ‘Early canid domestication: the farm fox experiment’, American Scientist 87 (March–April), 160–9.

8. Brian Hare, Victoria Wobber and Richard Wrangham (2012), ‘The self-domestication hypothesis: evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression’, Animal Behavior, 83, 573–85.

9. B. Hare (2007), ‘From Nonhuman to Human Mind. What Changed and Why’, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 60–4.

10. Adam Brumm, Gitte M. Jensen, Gert D. van den Bergh, Michael J. Morwood, Iwan Kurniawan, Fachroel Aziz and Michael Storey (2010), ‘Hominins on Flores, Indonesia, by one million years ago’, Nature, 464, 748–52.

11. Alan Simmons (2012), ‘Mediterranean island voyages’, Science, 338, 895–7.

12. Adam Powell, Stephen Shennan and Mark G. Thomas (2009), ‘Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior’, Science, 324, 1298–1301.

13. H. Zheng, S. Yan, Z. Qin and L. Jin (2012), ‘MtDNA analysis of global populations support that major population expansions began before Neolithic Time’, Scientific Reports, 745; DOI:10.1038/srep00745.


CHAPTER 1

1. Dan Wolpert opens with this question in his TED talk: http://www.ted.com/talks/daniel_wolpert_the_real_reason_for_brains.html

2. The example of the sea squirt is given by a variety of authors but most notably Rodolfo R. Llinás (2001), I of the Vortex: From Neurons to Self, MIT Press.

3. F. de Waal (2013), The Bonobo and the Atheist: In Search of Humanism Among the Primates, W. W. Norton & Company.

4. Jane Goodall (1986), The Chimpanzees of the Gombe: Patterns of Behavior, Cambridge: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

5. M. Nakamichi, E. Kato, Y. Kojima and N. Itoigawa (1998), ‘Carrying and washing of grass roots by free-ranging Japanese macaques at Katsuyama’, Folia Primatologica International Journal of Primatology, 69, 35–40.

6. Lydia V. Luncz, Roger Mundry and Christophe Boesch (2012), ‘Evidence for cultural differences between neighboring chimpanzee communities’, Current Biology, 22, 922–6.

7. Richard Dawkins (1976), The Selfish Gene, Oxford University Press.

8. Richard Dawkins (1996), The Blind Watchmaker: Why the Evidence of Evolution Reveals a Universe without Design, New York: Norton & Company.

9. M. E. J. Newman and R. G. Palmer (1999), ‘Models of Extinction: A Review’, Santa Fe Institute working paper, http://www.santafe.edu/media/workingpapers/99–08-061.pdf.

10. The ‘environmental complexity hypothesis’ argues that one of the driving forces for developing intelligence supported by larger brains was the need to adapt to variable environments.

Grove, M. (2011), ‘Change and variability in Plio-Pleistocene climates: Modelling the hominin response’, Journal of Archaeological Science, 38, 3038–47.

11. X. H. Zhu, H. Qiao, F. Du, Q. Xiong, X. Liu, X. Zhang, K. Ugurbil and W. Chen (2012), ‘Quantitative imaging of energy expenditure in human brain’, Neuroimage, 60, 2107–17.

12. D. Attwell and S. B. Laughlin (2001), ‘An energy budget for signaling in the grey matter of the brain’, Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 21, 1133–45.

13. L. Marino (1998), ‘A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans and anthropoid primates’, Brain, Behavior, and Evolution, 51, 230–8.

14. I. Loudon (1986), Deaths in childbed from the eighteenth century to 1935’, Medical History, 30, 1–41.

15. J. DeSilva and J. Lesnik (2006), ‘Chimpanzee neonatal brain size: Implications for brain growth in Homo erectus’, Journal of Human Evolution, 51, 207–12.

16. A. Portmann (1969), ‘Biologische Fragmente zu einer Lehre vom Menschen [A Zoologist Looks at Humankind] (Schwabe, Basel, Germany); trans. J. Schaefer (1990), New York: Columbia University Press.

17. C. D. Bluestone (2005), ‘Humans are born too soon: impact on pediatric otolaryngology’, International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 69, 1–8.

18. J. H. Kaas (2005), ‘From mice to men: the evolution of the large, complex human brain’, Journal of Bioscience, 30, 155–65.

19. Holly M. Dunsworth, Anna G. Warrener, Terrence Deacon, Peter T. Ellison and Herman Pontzer (2012), ‘Metabolic hypothesis for human altriciality’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 109, 15212–16.

20. R. D. Martin (1996), ‘Scaling of the mammalian brain: The maternal energy hypothesis’, News in Physiological Science, 11, 149–56.

21. K. R. Rosenberg and W. R. Trevathan (2003), ‘The Evolution of Human Birth’, Scientific American, May: 80–85.

22. Milton, Katharine (2000), ‘Diet and Primate Evolution’ in Alan Goodman, Darna Dufour and Gretel Pelto (eds), Nutritional Anthropology: Biocultural Perspectives on Food and Nutrition, Mountain View, CA: Mayfield Publishing Company, 46–54.

23. G. T. Frost (1980), ‘Tool behavior and the origins of laterality’, Journal of Human Evolution, 9, 447–59.

24. John Allen (2009), The Lives of the Brain: Human Evolution and the Organ of Mind, Belknap Harvard

25. The Leakeys reported the discovery of fossils at Koobi Fora, near Lake Turkana in Kenya, that indicate that three separate hominid species co-existed as early as 2 million years ago. M.G. Leakey, F. Spoor, M.C. Dean, C.S. Feibel, S.C. Antón, C. Kiarie and L.N. Leakey (2012), ‘New fossils from Koobi Fora in northern Kenya confirm taxonomic diversity in early Homo’, Nature, 488, 201– 204.

However, recent discoveries in the Georgian village of Dmanisi of a variety of skull shapes from Homo erectus dating from 2 million years ago suggest much less evidence for distinct species of hominids evolving in Africa based on different skull shapes.

David Lordkipanidze, Marcia S. Ponce de León, Ann Margvelashvili, Yoel Rak, G. Philip Rightmire, Abesalom Vekua and Christoph P. E. Zollikofer (2013), ‘A Complete Skull from Dmanisi, Georgia, and the Evolutionary Biology of Early Homo’, Science, Vol. 342 no. 6156, 326–31.

26. I. McDougall, F. H. Brown and J. G. Fleagle (2005), ‘Stratigraphic placement and age of modern humans from Kibish, Ethiopia’, Nature, 433, 733–6.

27. R. L. Cann, M. Stoneking and A. C. Wilson (1987), ‘Mitochondrial DNA and human evolution’, Nature, 325, 31–6.

28. Annalee Newitz (2013), A long anthropological debate may be on the cusp of resolution, http://io9.com/a-long-anthropological-debate-may-be-on-the-cusp-of-res-512864731 (Interview with Ian Tattersall)

29. University of Montreal (2011, July 18), ‘Non-Africans are part Neanderthal, genetic research shows’, Science Daily. Retrieved July 4, 2013, from http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2011/07/110718085329.htm

30. R. I. M. Dunbar and S. Shultz (2007), ‘Evolution in the social brain’, Science, 317, 1344–7.

31. J. B. Silk (2007), ‘Social components of fitness in primate groups’, Science, 317, 1347–51.

32. M. Gutison et al. (2012), ‘Derived vocalizations of geladas (Theropithecus gelada) and the evolution of vocal complexity in primates’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of Biological Sciences, 367, 1847–59.

33. Nicola Clayton (2012), ‘Corvid cognition: Feathered apes’, Nature, 484, 453–4.

34. Chris Stringer (2011), The Origin of our Species, London: Allen Lane.

35. H. Zheng, S. Yan, Z. Qin and L. Jin (2012), ‘MtDNA analysis of global populations support that major population expansions began before Neolithic Time’, Sci. Rep. 2, 745; DOI:10.1038/srep00745.

36. S. Mithen, (1996), The Prehistory of the Mind: A Search for the Origins of Art, Religion and Science, London: Thames and Hudson.

37. Nicholas Humphrey (1983),Consciousness Regained, Oxford University Press.

38. Frans de Waal (2007), Chimpanzee Politics: Power and Sex Among Apes (25th Anniversary ed.), Baltimore, MD: JHU Press.

39. D. G. Premack and G. Woodruff (1978), ‘Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?’,Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–26.

40. Steven Pinker (1994), The Language Instinct, New York: Morrow.

41. R. I. M. Dunbar (1996), Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language, London: Faber & Faber.

42. S. Pinker and P. Bloom (1990), ‘Natural language and natural selection’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 707–84.

43. A. A. Ghazanfar and D. Rendall (2008), ‘Evolution of human vocal production’, Current Biology, 18, 457–60.

44. K. S. Lashley (1951), ‘The problem of serial order in behavior’ in L. A. Jefress (ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior, New York: Wiley, 112–46.

45. Noam Chomsky (1986), Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin and Use, New York: Praeger.

46. Steve Pinker (1994), The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language, New York: William Morrow & Co.

47. Leda Cosmides and John Tooby (1994), ‘Origins of domain specificity: The evolution of functional organization’ in L. Hirshfeld and S. A. Gelman (eds), Mapping the Mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture, New York: Cambridge University Press.

48. Maciej Chudek, Patricia Brosseau-Laird, Susan Birch and Joseph Henrich (2013), ‘Culture-Gene Coevolutionary Theory and Children’s Selective Social Learning’ in M. R. Banaji and S. A. Gelman (eds), Navigating the Social World. What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us, New York: Oxford University Press.

49. Mike Tomasello (2009), Why We Cooperate, Boston: Boston Review.

50. Felix Warneken, Brian Hare, Alicia P. Melis, Daniel Hanus and Michael Tomasello (2007), ‘Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children’, PLoS Biol 5(7): e184. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0050184.

51. Judith M. Burkart, Ernst Fehr, Charles Efferson and Carel P. van Schaik (2007), ‘Other-regarding preferences in a non-human primate: Common marmosets provision food altruistically’, Proceedings of the National Association of Sciences, 104, 19762–6.


CHAPTER 2

1. John Locke, (1690), An Essay Concerning Human Understanding, New York: E. P. Dutton, 1947.

2. William James (1890), Principles of Psychology, New York: Henry Holt.

3. Immanual Kant (1781), Critique of Pure Reason, trans. J. M. D. Meiklejohn, The Electronic Classics Series, ed. Jim Manis, PSU-Hazleton, Hazleton, PA.

4. J. M. Fuster (2003), Cortex and Mind, New York: Oxford University Press

5. F. A. C. Azevedo et al. (2009), ‘Equal numbers of neuronal and nonneuronal cells make the human brain an isometrically scaled-up primate brain’, Journal of Comparative Neurology, 513, 532–541.

This is the most recent analysis of the human neural architecture. They estimated that there were 85 billion non-neuronal cells and 86 billion neuronal cells.

6. R. C. Knickmeyer, S. Gouttard, C. Kang, D. Evans, K. Wilber, J. K. Smith et al. (2008), ‘A structural MRI study of human brain development from birth to 2 years’, Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 12176–82.

7. Gregory Z. Tau and Bradley S. Peterson (2010), ‘Normal development of brain circuits’, Neuropsychopharmacology Reviews, 35, 147–68.

8. David A. Drachman (2005), ‘Do we have brain to spare?’, Neurology, 64, 2004–5.

9. This is a paraphrase of Donald Hebb’s rules of neuronal learning and synaptic plasticity.

D. O. Hebb (1949), The Organization of Behavior, New York: Wiley & Sons.

10. Elizabeth S. Spelke, (2000), ‘Core knowledge’, American Psychologist, 55, 1233–43.

11. Valerie A. Kuhlmeier, Paul Bloom and Karen Wynn (2004), ‘Do 5-month-old infants see humans as material objects?’, Cognition, 94, 95–103.

12. Aina Puce and David Perrett (2003), ‘Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion’, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B (2003) 358, 435–45.

13. Virginia Slaughter, Michelle Heron-Delaney and Tamara Christie (2012), ‘Developing expertise in human body perception’ in V. Slaughter and C.A Brownell (eds), Early Development of Body Representations. Cambridge Studies in Cognitive and Perceptual Development 13, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 207–26.

14. F. Simion, L. Regolin and H. Bulf (2008), ‘A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 105, 809–13.

15. A. J. DeCasper and M. J. Spence (1986), ‘Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns’ perception of speech sounds’, Infant Behavior & Development, 9, 133–50.

16. Aidan Macfarlane (1975), ‘Olfaction in the development of social preferences in the human neonate’ in A. Macfarlane (ed.), Parent-Infant Interactions, Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 103–17.

17. Dare A. Baldwin (2013), ‘Redescribing Action’ in M. R. Banaji and S. A. Gelman (eds), Navigating the Social World. What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us, New York: Oxford University Press.

18. D. A. Baldwin, J. A. Baird, M. Saylor and M. A. Clark (2001), ‘Infants parse dynamic human action’, Child Development, 72, 708–17.

19. Margaret Legerstee (1992), ‘A review of the animate-inanimate distinction in infancy. Implications for models of social and cognitive knowing’, Early Development and Parenting, 1, 59–67.

20. F. Heider and M. Simmel (1944), ‘An experimental study of apparent behavior’, American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–59.

21. Dan C. Dennett (1971), ‘Intentional systems’, Journal of Philosophy, 68, 87–106.

22. Val Kuhlmeier, Karen Wynn and Paul Bloom (2003), ‘Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds’, Psychological Science, 14, 402–8.

23. J. Kiley Hamlin, Karen Wynn and Paul Bloom (2010), ‘Three-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations’, Developmental Science, 13, 923–9.

24. A. L. Yarbus (1967), Eye Movements and Vision (trans. B. Haigh), New York: Plenum Press.

25. Stewart Guthrie (1993), Faces in the Clouds: A New Theory of Religion, Oxford University Press.

26. Nancy Kanwisher, Josh McDermott and Marvin M. Chun (1997), ‘The Fusiform Face Area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception’, Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302–11.

27. Michael Argyle and Janet Dean (1965), ‘Eye-contact, distance and affiliation’, Sociometry, 28, 289–304.

28. A. Frischen, A. P. Bayliss, S. P. Tipper (2007), ‘Gaze cueing of attention: visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences’, Psychological Bulletin, 133, 694–724.

29. Bruce Hood, Doug Willen and Jon Driver (1998), ‘An eye direction detector triggers shifts of visual attention in human infants’, Psychological Science, 9, 53–6.

30. M. Von Grünau and C. Anston (1995), ‘The detection of gaze direction: a stare-in-the-crowd effect’, Perception, 24, 1297–1313.

31. Reginald B. J. Adams, Heather L. Gordon, Abigail A. Baird, Nalini Ambady and Robert E. Kleck (2003), ‘Effects of gaze on amygdala sensitivity to anger and fear faces’, Science, 300, 1536.

32. Teresa Farroni, Gergely Csibra, Francesca Simion and Mark H. Johnson (2002), ‘Eye contact detection in humans from birth’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99, 9602–5.

33. S. M. J. Hains and D. W. Muir (1996), ‘Effects of stimulus contingency in infant–adult interactions’, Infant Behavior & Development, 19, 49–61.

34. M. Argyle and M. Cook, Gaze and Mutual Gaze (1976), Cambridge University Press.

35. H. Akechi, A. Senju, H. Uibo, Y. Kikuchi, T. Hasegawa et al. (2013), ‘Attention to Eye Contact in the West and East: Autonomic Responses and Evaluative Ratings’, PLoS ONE 8(3): e59312. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059312.

36. J. Kellerman, J. Lewis and J. D. Laird (1989), ‘Looking and loving: The effects of mutual gaze on feelings of romantic love’, Journal of Research in Personality, 23, 145–61.

37. E. Nurmsoo, S. Einav and B. M. Hood (2012), ‘Best friends: children use mutual gaze to identify friendships in others’, Developmental Science, 15, 417–25.

38. M. Bateson, D. Nettle and G. Roberts (2006), ‘Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting’, Biology Letters, 2, 412–14.

D. Francey and R. Bergmu¨ller (2012), Images of eyes enhance investments in a real-life public good. PLoS ONE 7, e37397.

Kate L. Powell, Gilbert Roberts and Daniel Nettle (2012), ‘Eye images increase charitable donations: Evidence from an opportunistic field experiment in a supermarket’, Ethology, 118, 1–6.

M. Ernest-Jones, D. Nettle and M. Bateson (2011), ‘Effects of eye images on everyday cooperative behavior: a field experiment’, Evol. Hum. Behav. 32, 172–8.

39. Mike Tomasello (2009), ‘Why We Cooperate’, Boston Review.

40. M. Tomasello and M. J. Farrar (1986), ‘Joint attention and early language’, Child Development, 57, 1454–63.

41. G. Butterworth (2003), ‘Pointing is the royal road to language for babies’ in S. Kita (ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 9–33.

42. Others believe that apes share all the same communicative gestures and joint attention as humans. David A. Leavens (2012), ‘Joint attention: twelve myths’ in Joint attention: New developments in Psychology, Philosophy of Mind, and Social Neuroscience, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, pp. 43–72.

43. Anne Fernald and T. Simon (1984), ‘Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns’, Developmental Psychology, 20, 104–13.

44. Andrew N. Meltzoff and Rechele Brooks (2001), ‘ “Like me” as a building block for understanding other minds: bodily acts, attention, and intention’ in Betram F. Malle and Dare Baldwin (eds), Intentions and Intentionality: Foundations of Social Cognition, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 171–91.

45. Rod Parker-Rees (2007), ‘Liking to be liked: imitation, familiarity and pedagogy in the first years of life’, Early Years, 27, 3–17.

46. Andrew N. Meltzoff (1995), ‘Apprehending the intentions of others. Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children’, Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–50.

47. G. Gergely, H. Bekkering and I. Kiraly (2002), ‘Rational imitation in preverbal infants’, Nature, 415, 755.

48. V. Horner and A. Whiten (2005), ‘Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens)’, Animal Cognition, 8, 164–81.

49. Derek E. Lyons, Andrew G. Young, and Frank C. Keil (2007), ‘The hidden structure of over imitation’, Proceedings of the National Academy, 104, 19751–6.

50. P. A. Herrmann, C. H. Legare, P. L. Harris and H. Whitehouse (2013), ‘Stick to the script: The effect of witnessing multiple actors on children’s imitation’, Cognition, 129, 536–43.

51. C. H. Legare and P. A. Herrmann (2013), ‘Cognitive consequences and constraints on reasoning about ritual’, Religion, Brain and Behavior, 3, 63–5.

52. A. Phillips, H. M. Wellman and E. S. Spelke (2002), ‘Infants’ ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action’, Cognition, 85, 53–78.

53. S. Itakura, H. Ishida, T. Kanda, Y. Shimada, H. Ishiguro et al. (2008), ‘How to build an intentional android: Infant imitation of a robot’s goal-directed actions’, Infancy, 13, 519–32.

54. R. W. Byrne and A. Whiten (eds) (1988), ‘Machiavellian Intelligence. Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect’ in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans, Oxford: Oxford University Press.

55. A. Gopnik and J. W. Astington (1988), ‘Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance reality distinction’, Child Development, 59, 26–37.

56. Jean Piaget and Barbel Inhelder (1956), The Child’s Conception of Space, London: Routledge & Keegan Paul.

57. Hans Wimmer and Josef Perner (1983), ‘Beliefs about beliefs: Representations and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception’, Cognition, 13, 103–28.

58. Kristine H. Onishi and Renée Baillargeon (2005), ‘Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs’, Science, 308, 255–8.

59. Carla Krachun, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call and Michael Tomasello (2009), ‘A competitive nonverbal false belief task for children and apes’, Developmental Science, 12, 521–35.

60. Susan A. Birch and Paul Bloom (2007), ‘The curse of knowledge in reasoning about false beliefs’, Psychological Science, 18, 382–6.

61. Ian Apperly (in press), ‘Can theory of mind grow up? Mindreading in adults, and its implications for the development and neuroscience of mindreading’ in S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg and M. Lombardo (eds), Understanding Other Minds (third edition).

62. Lawrence A. Hirschfeld (2013), ‘The Myth of Mentalizing and the Primacy of Folk Sociology’ in M. R. Banaji and S. A. Gelman (eds), Navigating the Social World, New York: Oxford University Press.

63. David Liu and Kimberly E. Vanderbilt (2013), ‘Children Learn From and About Variability Between People’ in M. R. Banaji and S. A. Gelman (eds), Navigating the Social World. What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us, New York: Oxford University Press.

64. C. H. Legare (2012), ‘Exploring explanation: Explaining inconsistent information guides hypothesis-testing behavior in young children’, Child Development, 83, 173–85.


CHAPTER 3

1. The exact cause of Joseph Merrick’s condition is still unresolved but candidate diseases of Proteus Syndrome and neurofibromatosis Type I have been suggested.

2. M. Howell and P. Ford (1992) [1980], The True History of the Elephant Man (third edition), London: Penguin.

3. I. Stevenson (1992), ‘A new look at maternal impressions: an analysis of 50 published cases and reports of two recent examples’, Journal of Scientific Exploration, 6, 353–373.

4. Clarence Maloney (1976), The Evil Eye, New York: Columbia University Press.

5. E. A. Kensinger and D. L. Schacter (2005), ‘Emotional content and reality monitoring ability: fMRI evidence for the influence of encoding processes’, Neuropsychologica, 43, 1429–43.

6. M. Joëls, Z. Pu, O. Wiegert, M. S. Oitzl and H. J. Krugers (2006), ‘Learning under stress: how does it work?’, Trends in Cognitive Science, 10, 152–8.

7. R. Rachel Yehuda, Stephanie Mulherin Engel, Sarah R. Brand, Jonathan Seckl, Sue M. Marcus, and Gertrud S. Berkowitz (2005), ‘Transgenerational effects of posttraumatic stress disorder in babies of mothers exposed to the World Trade Center attacks during pregnancy’, Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 90, 4115–18.

8. Quote from Discover Magazine article published online 14 October 2010: http://discovermagazine.com/2010/oct/11-how-did-9–11-affect-pregnant-mothers-children

9. J. Kagan, (1994), Galen’s Prophecy: Temperament in Human Nature, New York: Basic Books.

10. K. J. Saudino (2005), ‘Behavioral genetics and child temperament’, Journal of Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics, 26, 214–33.

11. N. A. Fox, H. A. Henderson, K. H. Rubin, S. D. Calkins and L. A. Schmidt (2001), ‘Continuity and discontinuity of behavioural inhibition and exuberance: Psychophysiological and behavioural influences across the first four years of life’, Child Development, 72, 1–21.

12. J. Bowlby (1969), Attachment, Attachment and Loss, Vol. 1, London: Hogarth Press.

13. K. Lorenz (1943), ‘Die Angebornen Formen mogicher Erfahrung’, Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 5, 233–409.

14. M. H. Johnson & J. Morton (1991), Biology and Cognitive Development: The Case of Face Recognition, Oxford: Blackwell.

15. P. S. Zeskind and B. M. Lester, (2001), ‘Analysis of infant crying’ in L. T. Singer and P. S. Zeskind (eds), Biobehavioral Assessment of the Infant, New York: Guilford, pp. 149–66.

16. E. E. Maccoby (1980), Social Development: Psychological growth and the parent-child relationship, New York: Harcourt Brace Janovich.

17. H. F. Harlow (1958), ‘The nature of love’, American Psychologist, 13, 573–685.

18. M. Rutter, T. G. O’Connor and The English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) Study Team (2004), ‘Are there biological programming effects for psychological development? Findings from a study of Romanian adoptees’, Developmental Psychology, 40, 81–94.

19. J. A. Whitson and A. D. Galinsky (2008), ‘Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception’, Science, 322, 115–17.

20. T. V. Salomons, T. Johnstone, M. Backonja and R. J. Davidson (2004), ‘Perceived controllability modulates the neural response to pain’, Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 7199–203.

21. L. Murray, A. Fiori-Cowley, R. Hooper and P. Cooper (1996), ‘The impact of postnatal depression and associated adversity on early mother-infant interactions and later infant outcome’, Child Development, 67, 2512–26.

22 C. M. Pariante and A. H. Miller (2001), ‘Glucocorticoid receptors in major depression: Relevance to pathophysiology and treatment’, Biological Psychiatry, 49, 391–404.

23. C. S. de Kloet, E. Vermetten, E. Geuze, A. Kavelaars, C. J. Heijnen and H. G. M. Westenberg (2006), ‘Assessment of HPA-axis function in posttraumatic stress disorder: pharmacological and non-pharmacological challenge tests, a review’, Journal of Psychiatric Research, 40, 550–67.

24. A. K. Pesonen, K. Räikkönen, K. Feldt, K. Heinonen, C. Osmond, D. I. Phillips, D. J. Barker, J. G. Eriksson and E. Kajantie (2010), ‘Childhood separation experience predicts HPA axis hormonal responses in late adulthood: a natural experiment of World War II’, Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 758–67.

25. S. Clarke, D. J. Wittwer, D. H. Abbott and M. L. Schneider (1994), ‘Long-term effects of prenatal stress on HPA axis activity in juvenile rhesus monkeys’, Developmental Neurobiology, 27, 257–69.

26. Alice Graham, Phil Fisher and Jennifer Pfeifer (2013), ‘What sleeping babies hear: A functional MRI study of interparental conflict and infants’ emotion processing’, Psychological Science, 24, 782–9.

27. L. Trut, I. Oskina and A. Kharlamova (2009), ‘Animal evolution during domestication: the domesticated fox as a model’, BioEssays, 31, 349–60.

28. Molly Crockett (2009), ‘Values, Empathy, and Fairness across Social Barriers’, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1167, 76–86.

29. L. N. Trut, I. Z. Plyusnina and I. N. Oskina (2004), ‘An experiment on fox domestication and debatable issues of evolution of the dog’, Russian Journal of Genetics, 40, 644–55.

30. This is known as the James–Lange theory after Carl Lange developed William James’s initial proposal.

C. G. Lange and W. James (1922), The Emotions, Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.

31. This alternative to the James–Lange theory was the Cannon–Bard theory after W. B. Cannon (1929), ‘The James–Lange theory of emotion: A critical examination and alternative theory’, American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106–24.

P. Bard (1934), ‘On emotional experience after decortication with some remarks on theoretical views’, Psychological Review, 41, 309–29.

32. Joseph LeDoux (1998), The Emotional Brain, London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.

33. S. Schacter and J. E. Singer (1962), ‘Cognitive, social and psychological determinants of emotional state’, Psychological Review, 69, 379–99.

34. Ian Pento-Voak, Jamie Thomas, Suzanne Gage, Mary McMurran, Sarah McDonald and Marcus Munafo (2013), ‘Increasing recognition of happiness in ambiguous facial expressions reduces anger and aggressive behavior’, Psychological Science, 24, 688–97.

35. R. M. Sullivan, M. Landers, B. Yeaman and D. A. Wilson (2000), ‘Good memories of bad events in infancy: Ontogeny of conditioned fear and the amygdala’, Nature, 407, 38–9.

36. S. Moriceau and R. M. Sullivan (2006), ‘Maternal presence serves as a switch between learning fear and attraction in infancy’, Nature Neuroscience, 9, 1004–6.

37. Sarah L. Master, Naomi I. Eisenberger, Shelley E. Taylor, Bruce D. Naiboff, David Shirinyan and Matthew D. Leiberman (2009), ‘A picture’s worth: Partner photographs reduce experimentally induced pain’, Psychological Science, 20, 1316–18.

38. Dean Jensen (2006), The Lives and Loves of Daisy and Violet Hilton: A True Story of Conjoined Twins, Berkeley, CA: Ten Speed Press.

39. Judith Rich Harris (2006), No Two Alike: Human Nature and Human Individuality, W. W. Norton.

40. Guttal et al. (2012), ‘Cannibalism can drive the evolution of behavioural phase polyphenism in locusts’, Ecology Letters, 15, 1158–66.

41. David Sheldon Cohen, A. J. Tyrrell and Andrew P. Smith (1991), ‘Psychological stress and susceptibility to the common cold’, New England Journal of Medicine, 325, 606–12.

42. S. W. Cole, L. C. Hawkley, J. M. Arevalo, C. Y. Sung, R. M. Rose and J. T. Cacioppo (2007), ‘Social regulation of gene expression in human leukocytes’, Genome Biology, 8, R189.

43. Steve W. Cole (2009), ‘Social regulation of human gene expression’, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 132–7.

44. R. Simmons and R. Altwegg (2010), ‘Necks-for-sex or competing browsers? A critique of ideas on the evolution of the giraffe’, Journal of Zoology, 282, 6–12.

45. F. A. Champagne, D. D. Francis, A. Mar and M. J. Meaney (2003), ‘Naturally-occurring variations in maternal care in the rat as a mediating influence for the effects of environment on the development of individual differences in stress reactivity’, Physiology & Behavior, 79, 359–71.

46. D. D. Francis, J. Diorio, D. Liu and M. J. Meaney (1999), ‘Nongenomic transmission across generations in maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat’, Science, 286, 1155–8.

47. M. J. Meaney (2001), ‘The development of individual differences in behavioral and endocrine responses to stress’, Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 1161–92.

48. P. O. McGowan, M. Suderman, A. Sasaki, T. C. Huang, M. Hallett, M. J. Meaney et al. (2011), ‘Broad epigenetic signature of maternal care in the brain of adult rats’, PLoS ONE, 6, e14739.

49. P. O. McGowan, A. Sasaki, A. C. D’Alessio, S. Dymov, B. Labonte, M. Szyf, G. Turecki and M. J. Meaney (2009), ‘Epigenetic regulation of the glucocorticoid receptor in human brain associates with childhood abuse’, Nature Neuroscience 12, 342–8.

50. Marilyn J. Essex, W. Tom Boyce, Clyde Hertzman, Lucia L. Lam, Jeffrey M. Armstrong, Sarah M. Neumann and Michael S. Kobor (2013), ‘Epigenetic Vestiges of early developmental adversity: Childhood stress exposure and DNA methylation in adolescence’, Child Development, 84, 58–75.

51. H. G. Brunner, M. Nelen, X. O. Breakefield, H. H. Ropers and B. A. van Oost (1993), ‘Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A’, Science, 262, 578–80.

52. Ann Gibbons (2004), ‘Tracking the evolutionary history of a “warrior” gene’, Science, 304, 5672.

53. R. A. Lea, D. Hall, M. Green and C. K. Chambers, ‘Tracking the evolutionary history of the warrior gene in the South Pacific’, presented at the Molecular Biology and Evolution Conference in Auckland, June 2005, and the International Congress of Human Genetics, Brisbane, August 2006.

54. Rose McDermott, Dustin Tingley, Jonathan Cowden, Giovanni Frazzetto and Dominic D. P. Johnson (2009), ‘Monoamine oxidase A gene (MAOA) predicts behavioral aggression following provocation’, Proceedings of the National Academy. www.pnas.org_cgi_doi_10.1073_pnas.0808376106

55. Ed Yong (2010), ‘Dangerous DNA: The truth about the “warrior gene” ’, New Scientist, 7 April 2010.

56. A. Caspi, J. McClay, T. E. Moffitt, J. Mill, J. Martin, I. W. Craig, A. Taylor, and R. Poulton (2002), ‘Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children’, Science, 297, 851–4.


CHAPTER 4

1. E. Macphail (1982), Brain and Intelligence in Vertebrates, Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.

2. R. A. Barton and C. Venditti (2013), ‘Human frontal lobes are not relatively large’, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110, 9001–6.

3. Jeffrey Rogers et al. (2010), ‘On the genetic architecture of cortical folding and brain volume in primates’, NeuroImage, 53, 1103–8.

4. Kate Teffer and Katerina Semendeferi (2012), ‘Human prefrontal cortex: Evolution, development, and pathology’ in M. A. Hofman and D. Falk (eds), Progress in Brain Research, vol. 195, Elsevier.

5. J. Hill, T. Inder, J. Neil, D. Dierker, J. Harwell and D. Van Essen (2010), ‘Similar patterns of cortical expansion during human development and evolution’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 13135–40.

6. Xiling Liu, Mehmet Somel, Lin Tang et al. (2012), ‘Extension of cortical synaptic development distinguishes humans from chimpanzees and macaques’, Genome Research published online 2 February 2012: doi:10.1101/gr.127324.111

7. Robert W. Thatcher (1992), ‘Cyclic cortical reorganization during early childhood’, Brain and Cognition, 20, 24–50.

8. G. Kochanska, K. C. Coy, and K. T. Murray (2001), ‘The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life’, Child Development, 72, 1091–111.

9. Dan Gilbert (2007), Stumbling Upon Happiness, Perennial.

10. W. A. Roberts (2002), ‘Are animals stuck in time?’, Psychological Bulletin, 128, 473–89.

11. N. J. Mulcahy and J. Call (2010), ‘Apes save tools for future use’, Science, 312, 1038–9.

12. T. Suddendorf and J. Busby (2003), ‘Mental time travel in animals?’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 391–5.

13. T. Suddendorf and J. Busby (2005), ‘Making decisions with the future in mind: Developmental and comparative identification of mental time travel’, Learning and Motivation, 36, 110–25.

14. Christopher M. Filley (2010), ‘The frontal lobes’ in Michael J. Aminoff, François Boller and Dick F. Swaab (eds), History of Neurology, Elsevier B.V., pp. 557–70.

15. D. O. Hebb (1977), ‘Wilder Penfield: his legacy to neurology. The frontal lobe’, Canadian Medical Association Journal, 116, 1373–4.

16. Philip David Zelazo and Stephanie M. Carlson (2012), ‘Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity’, Child Development Perspectives, 6, 354–60.

17. Yuko Munakata, Seth A. Herd, Christopher H. Chatham, Brendan E. Depue, Marie T. Banich and Randall C. O’Reilly (2011), ‘A unified framework for inhibitory control’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 453–9.

18. A. D. Smith, I. D. Gilchrist and B. M. Hood (2005), ‘Children’s search behaviour in large-scale space: Developmental components of exploration’, Perception, 34, 1221–9.

19. Brenda Milner (1963), ‘Effect of Different Brain Lesions on Card Sorting’, Archives of Neurology, 9, 90–100.

20. Adele Diamond (1991), ‘Neuropsychological insights into the meaning of object concept development’ in S.Carey and R. Gelman (eds), The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 67–110.

21. John Ridley Stroop (1935), ‘Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions’, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–62.

22. N. Raz, (2000), ‘Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: Integration of structural and functional findings’ in F. I. Criak and T. A. Salthouse (eds), Handbook of Aging and Cognition, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 1–90.

23. P. W. Burgess and R. L. Wood (1990), ‘Neuropsychology of behaviour disorders following brain injury’ in R. L. Wood (ed.), Neurobehavioural sequelae of traumatic brain injury, New York: Taylor and Francis, pp. 110–33.

24. M. Macmillan (2000), An Odd Kind of Fame: Stories of Phineas Gage, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

25. Alexander’s story is reported here: http://www.dailymail.co.uk/health/article-393938/The-freak-accident-left-son-obsessed-sex.html

Here is the video of the marathon incident: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ELsGvt4Lsjo

26. S. J. Blakemore (2012), ‘Imaging brain development: The adolescent brain’, Neuroimage, 61, 397–406.

27. J. A. Fugelsang and K. N. Dunbar (2005), ‘Brain-based mechanisms underlying complex causal thinking’, Neuropsychologia, 43, 1204–13.

28. Terrie Moffitt et al. (2011), ‘A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 108, 2693–8.

29. K. Meiers (2002), Problem Schulfähigkeit. Grundschule 5, 10–12

30. Walter Mischel, Ebbe B. Ebbesen and Antonette Raskoff Zeiss (1972), ‘Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 204–18.

31. Walter Mischel, Yuichi Shoda and Monica L. Rodriguez (1989), ‘Delay of gratification in children’, Science, 244, 933–8.

32. Erik Erikson (1963), Childhood and Society, New York: Norton, p. 262.

33. Susan Crockenberg and Cindy Litman (1990), ‘Autonomy as competence in 2-year-olds: Maternal correlates of child defiance, compliance, and self-assertion’, Developmental Psychology, 26, 961–971.

34. Lisa Cameron, N. Erkal, L. Gangdharan and X. Meng (2013), ‘Little Emperors: Behavioral impacts of China’s one-child policy’, Science, 339, 953–7.

35. Celeste Kidd, Holly Palmeri and Richard N. Aslin (2013), ‘Rational snacking: Young children’s decision-making on the marshmallow task is moderated by beliefs about environmental reliability’, Cognition, 126, 109–14.

36. L. Michaelson, A. dela Vega, C. H. Chatham and Y. Munakata (2013), ‘Delaying gratification depends on social trust’, Frontiers in Psychology, 4:355.doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00355

37. I. J. Toner, L. P. Moore and B. A. Emmons (1980), ‘The effect of being labeled on subsequent self-control in children’, Child Development, 51, 618–21.

38. Richard H. Thaler and Cass R. Sunstein (2009, updated edition), Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness, New York: Penguin.

39. L. A. Liikkanen (2008), ‘Music in every mind: Commonality of involuntary musical imagery’ in: K. Miyazaki, Y. Hiraga, M. Adachi, Y. Nakajima and M. Tsuzaki (eds), Proceedings of the 10th international conference on music perception and cognition (ICMPC10), 408–412. Sapporo, Japan.

40. D. M. Wegner, D. J Schneider, S. R. Carter and T. L. White (1987), ‘Paradoxical effects of thought suppression’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5–13.

41. C. Neil Macrae, Galen V. Bodenhausen, Alan B. Milne, Jolanda Jetten (1994), ‘Out of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 808–17.

42. Roger L. Albin and Jonathan W. Mink (2006), ‘Recent advances in Tourette Syndrome research’, Trends in Neurosciences, 39, 175–82.

43. I have Tourette’s but Tourette’s doesn’t have me (2005). http://www.imdb.com/title/tt0756661/quotes

44. I. Osborn (1998), Tormenting Thoughts and Secret Rituals: The Hidden Epidemic of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (New York, NY: Dell).

45. A. M. Graybiel and S. L. Rauch (2000), ‘Toward a neurobiology of obsessive compulsive disorder’, Neuron, 28, 343–7.

46. Roy F. Baumeister and John Tierney (2011), Willpower: Why self-control is the secret to success, Penguin.

47. R. F. Baumeister, E. Bratslavsky, M. Muraven and D. M. Tice (1998), ‘Self- control depletion: Is the active self a limited resource?’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–65.

48. Wilhelm Hofmann, Roy F. Baumeister, Georg Förster and Kathleen D. Vohs (2012), ‘Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 1318–35.


CHAPTER 5

1. William Golding (1954), Lord of the Flies, London: Faber & Faber.

2. Taken from Golding’s acceptance speech for the Nobel Prize in Literature in 1983.

3. Laura Manuel (2006), ‘Relationship of personal authoritarianism with parenting styles’, Psychological Reports, 98, 193–8.

4. Steve Pinker (2012), The Better Angels of Our Nature: A History of Violence and Humanity, London: Penguin.

5. http://rendezvous.blogs.nytimes.com/2012/09/02/has-the-burqa-ban-worked-in-france/ International Herald Tribune article, September 2012, retrieved March 2013.

6. Marc D. Hauser (2006), Moral Minds: How Nature Designed Our Universal Sense of Right and Wrong, New York: Harper Collins.

7. Valerie Kuhlmeier, Karen Wynn and Paul Bloom (2003), ‘Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds’, Psychological Science, 14, 402–8.

8. J. Kiley Hamlin, Karen Wynn and Paul Bloom (2007), ‘Social evaluation by preverbal infants’, Nature, 450, 557–9.

9. J. Kiley Hamlin, Karen Wynn, Paul Bloom and Neha Mahajan (2011), ‘How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others’, Proceedings of the National Academy, 108, 19931–6.

10. K.A. Dunfield and V.A. Khulmeier (2010), ‘Intention-mediated selective helping in infancy’, Psychological Science, 21, 523–7.

11. V. A. Khulmeier (2013), ‘Disposition attribution in infancy’ in M. R. Banaji and S. A. Gelman (eds), Navigating the Social World. What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us, New York: Oxford University Press.

12. Paul Bloom (2013), Just Babies: The Origins of Good and Evil. New York: Crown.

13. C. U. Shantz (1987), ‘Conflicts between children’, Child Development, 58, 283–305.

14. D. F. Hay and H. S. Ross (1982), ‘The social nature of early conflict’, Child Development, 53, 105–13.

15. 2013: http://www.thesun.co.uk/sol/homepage/news/4977445/Man-dragged-50ft-along-road-after-trying-to-stop-car-theft.html

2012: http://www.newsnet5.com/dpp/news/local_news/oh_cuyahoga/car-thieves-crash-stolen-car-killing-owner-who-was-hanging-onto-the-hood

http://www.tulsaworld.com/article.aspx/Woman_who_died_trying_to_prevent_car_theft_remembered/20120310_11_a1_cutlin972357

16. William James (1890), Principles of Psychology, New York: Henry Holt, p. 291.

17. R. Belk (1988), ‘Possessions and the extended self’, Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 139–68.

18. J. E. Stake (2004), ‘The property “instinct” ’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 359, 1763–74.

19. M. Rodgon and S. Rashman (1976), ‘Expression of owner-owned relationships among holophrastic 14- to 32-month-old children’, Child Development, 47, 1219–22.

20. B. Hood and P. Bloom (2008), ‘Children prefer certain individuals over perfect duplicates’, Cognition, 106, 455–62.

21. H. Ross, C. Conant and M. Vickar (2011), Property rights and the resolution of social conflict, New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 132, 53–64.

22. F. Rossano, H. Rakoczy and M Tomasello (2011), ‘Young children’s understanding of violations of property rights’, Cognition, 121, 219–27.

23. O. Friedman, J. W. van de Vondervoort, M. A. Defeyter and K. R. Neary (2013), ‘First possession, history, and young children’s ownership judgments’, Child Development, 84, 1519–25.

24. http://www.cbc.ca/news/yourcommunity/2013/02/bansky-graffiti-ripped-off-london-wall-put-on-auction-in-us.html

25. P. Kanngiesser, N. L. Gjersoe and B. M. Hood (2010), ‘Transfer of property ownership following creative labour in preschool children and adults’, Psychological Science, 21, 1236–41.

26. K. R. Olson and A. Shaw (2011), ‘ “No fair, copycat!”: what children’s response to plagiarism tells us about their understanding of ideas’, Developmental Science, 14, 431–9.

27. D. J. Turk, K. van Bussel, G. D. Waiter and C. N. Macrae (2011), ‘Mine and me: Exploring the neural basis of object ownership’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3657–68.

28. S. J. Cunningham, D. J Turk and C. N. Macrae (2008), ‘Yours or mine? Ownership and memory’, Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 312–18.

29. R. Thaler (1980), ‘Toward a positive theory of consumer choice’, Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 1, 39–60.

30. J. Heyman, Y. Orhun and D. Ariely (2004), ‘Auction fever: the effect of opponents and quasi-endowment on product valuations’, Journal of Interactive Marketing, 18, 7–21.

31. J. R. Wolf, H. R. Arkes and W. A. Muhanna (2008), ‘The power of touch: An examination of the effect of duration of physical contact on the valuation of objects’, Judgement and Decision Making, 3, 476–82.

32. D. Kahneman, J. L. Knetsch and R. H. Thaler (1991), ‘Anomalies: The endowment effect, loss aversion and status quo bias’, Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5, 193–206.

33. B. Knutson, G. E. Wimmer, S. Rick, N. G. Hollon, D. Prelec and G. Loewenstein (2008), ‘Neural antecedents of the endowment effect’, Neuron, 58, 814–22.

34. W. T. Harbaugh, K. Krause and L. Vesterlund (2001), ‘Are adults better behaved than children? Age, experience, and the endowment effect’, Economics Letters, 70, 175–81.

35. M. Wallendorf and E. J. Arnould (1988), ‘ “My favourite things”: A cross-cultural inquiry into object attachment, possessiveness, and social linkage’, Journal of Consumer Research, 14, 531–47.

36. Coren L. Apicella, Eduardo M. Azevedo, James H. Fowler and Nicholas A. Christakis (2013), ‘Evolutionary Origins of the Endowment Effect: Evidence from Hunter-Gatherers’, American Economic Review, 23 August 2013. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2255650 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2255650.

37. L. L. Birch and J. Billman (1986), ‘Preschool children’s food sharing with friends and acquaintances’, Child Development, 57, 387–95.

38. M. Gummerum, Y. Hanoch, M. Keller, K. Parsons and A. Hummel (2010), ‘Preschoolers’ allocations in the dictator game: The role of moral emotions’, Journal of Economic Psychology, 31, 25–34.

39. Ernst Fehr, Helen Bernhard and Bettina Rockenbach (2008), ‘Egalitarianism in young children’, Nature, 454, 1079–1084.

40. Katharina Hamann, Felix Warneken, Julia R. Greenberg and Michael Tomasello (2012), ‘Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not in chimpanzees’, Nature, 476, 328–31.

41. P. Blake and D. Rand (2010), ‘Currency value moderates equity preference among young children’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 210–18.

42. David Reinstein and Gerhard Riener (2012), ‘Reputation and influence in charitable giving: an experiment’, Theory and Decision, 72, 221–43.

43. F. Alpizar, F. Carlsson and O. Johansson-Stenman (2008), ‘Anonymity, reciprocity, and conformity: Evidence from voluntary contributions to a national park’, Journal of Public Economics, 92, 1047–1060.

44. K. L. Leimgruber, A. Shaw, L. R. Santos and K. R. Olson (2012), ‘Young Children Are More Generous When Others Are Aware of Their Actions’, PLoS ONE 7(10): e48292. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0048292

45. Felix Warneken and Michael Tomasello (2006), ‘Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees’, Science, 311, 1301–2.

46. M R. Lepper, D. Greene and R. E. Nisbett (1973), ‘Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with an extrinsic reward: A test of the “overjustification” hypothesis’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 129–37.

47. F. Warneken (2013), ‘What do children and chimpanzees reveal about human altruism?’ in M. R. Banaji and S. A. Gelman (eds), Navigating the Social World. What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us, New York: Oxford University Press.

48. Joan Silk, Commentary on Mike Tomasello (2009), Why We Cooperate, Boston: Boston Review.

49. F. Warneken, B. Hare, A. P. Melis, D. Haus and M. Tomasello, (2007), ‘Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children’, PLoS Biology, 5, 1414–20.

50. Judith M. Burkart, Ernst Fehr, Charles Efferson and Carel P. van Schaik (2007), ‘Other-regarding preferences in a non-human primate: Common marmosets provision food altruistically’, Proceedings of the National Academy, 104, 19762–6.

51. A. Ueno and T. Matsuzawa (2004), ‘Food transfer between chimpanzee mothers and their infants’, Primates, 45, 231–9.

52. William T. Harbaugh, Ulrich Mayr and Daniel R. Burghart (2007), ‘Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations’, Science, 316, 1622–5.

53. Ernst Fehr and Simon Gächter (2002), ‘Altruistic punishment in humans’, Nature, 415, 137–40.

54. W. Guth, R. Schmittberger and B. Schwarze (1982), ‘An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining’, Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 3, 367–88.

55. A. G. Sanfey, J. K. Rilling, J. A. Aronson, L. E. Nystrom and J. D. Cohen (2003), ‘The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game’, Science, 300, 1755–8.

56. D. Knoch, A. Pascual-Leone, K. Meyer, V. Treyer and E. Fehr (2006), ‘Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex’, Science, 314, 829–32.

57. K. Jensen, J. Call and M. Tomasello (2007), ‘Chimpanzees are maximizers in an ultimatum game’, Science, 318, 107–9.

58. Sarah F. Brosnan and Frans de Waal (2003), ‘Monkeys reject unequal pay’, Nature, 425, 297–9.

59. J. Bräuer, J. Call and M. Tomasello (2006), ‘Are apes really inequity averse?’, Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 273, 3123–8.

60. Dan Ariely (2008), Predictably Irrational, New York: HarperCollins.

61. John Nash (1951), ‘Non-cooperative Games’, Annals of Mathematics, 54, 286–95.

62. Richard Dawkins (1976), The Selfish Gene, Oxford University Press.

63. C. Adami and A. Hintze, ‘Evolutionary instability of zero determinant strategies demonstrates that winning is not everything’, Nature Communications, 4, 2193. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3193 (2013).

64. Paul Slovic (2007), ‘ “If I look at the mass I will never act”: Psychic numbing and genocide’, Judgement and Decision Making, 2, 79–95.

65. Karen E. Jenni and George Loewenstein (1997), ‘Explaining the “Identifiable victim effect” ’, Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 14, 235–57.

66. D. Västfjäll, E. Peters and P. Slovic (in preparation), ‘Representation, affect, and willingness-to-donate to children in need’, unpublished manuscript.

67. Leon R. Kass (1997), ‘The Wisdom of Repugnance’, The New Republic, 216, 17–26.

68. Jesse Bering (2013), Perv: The Sexual Deviant in All of Us, Scientific American/Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

69. Jonathan Haidt (2001), ‘The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgement’, Psychological Review, 108: 814–34.

70. J. Thomson (1985), ‘The Trolley Problem’, Yale Law Journal, 94, 1395–1415.

71. J. D. Greene, R. B. Sommerville, L. E. Nystrom, J. M. Darley and J. D. Cohen (2001), ‘An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgement’, Science, 293, 2105–8.

72. William B. Swann, Jr., Ángel Gómez, John F. Dovidio, Sonia Hart and Jolanda Jetten (2010), ‘Dying and killing for one’s group: Identity fusion moderates responses to intergroup versions of the trolley problem’, Psychological Science, 21, 1176–83.

73. Joshua Greene (2007), ‘The secret joke of Kant’s soul’, in W. Sinnott-Armstrong (ed.), Moral Psychology, Vol. 3: The Neuroscience of Morality: Emotion, Disease, and Development, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

74. Jean Piaget (1932/1965). The Moral Judgement of the Child, New York: Free Press.

75. Lawrence Kohlberg (1963), ‘Development of children’s orientation towards a moral order (Part I). Sequencing in the development of moral thought’, Vita Humana, 6, 11–36.

76. B. M. DePaulo and D. A. Kashy (1998), ‘Everyday lies in close and casual relationships’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 63–79.

77. B. M. DePaulo, D. A. Kashy, S. E. Kirkendol, M. M. Wyer and J. A. Epstein (1996), ‘Lying in everyday life’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 979–95.

78. William von Hippel and Robert Trivers (2011), ‘The evolution and psychology of self-deception’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 1–56.

79. Robert Trivers (1976), Foreword, in R. Dawkins, The Selfish Gene, Oxford University Press, pp. 19–20.

80. M. D. Alicke and C. Sedikides (2009), ‘Self-enhancement and self-protection: What they are and what they do’, European Review of Social Psychology, 20, 1–48.

81. Tali Sharot (2012), The Optimism Bias: A Tour of the Irrationally Positive Brain, London: Vintage.

82. C. Ward Struthers, Judy Eaton, Alexander G. Santelli, Melissa Uchiyama and Nicole Shirvani (2008), ‘The effects of attributions of intent and apology on forgiveness: When saying sorry may not help the story’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 983–92.

83. S. Harris, S. A. Sheth and M. S. Cohen (2007), ‘Functional Neuroimaging of belief, disbelief and uncertainty’, Annals of Neurology, 63, 141–7.

84. M. Main and C. George (1985), ‘Responses of young abused and disadvantaged toddlers to distress in age mates’, Developmental Psychology, 21, 407–12.

85. S. Johnson, C. S. Dweck and F. Chen (2007), ‘Evidence for infants’ internal working models of attachment’, Psychological Science, 18, 501–2.


CHAPTER 6

1. Shane Bauer, ‘Solitary in Iran nearly broke me. Then I went inside America’s prisons’, http://www.motherjones.com/politics/2012/10/solitary-confinement-shane-bauer, Mother Jones, December 2012, retrieved October 2013.

2. Nelson Mandela (1994), Long Walk to Freedom, London: Little Brown, p. 52.

3. Reuters, ‘U.S. Bureau of Prisons to review solitary confinement’, http://www.nytimes.com/reuters/2013/02/04/us/04reuters-usa-prisons-solitary.html?ref=solitaryconfinement, New York Times, February 2013, retrieved February 2013.

4. Joshua Foer and Michel Siffre (2008), ‘Caveman: An Interview with Michel Siffre’, http://www.cabinetmagazine.org/issues/30/foer.php, Cabinet Magazine, 1ssue 30.

5. Michel Siffre, ‘Six Months Alone in a Cave,’ National Geographic (March 1975), 426–435.

6. J. S. House, K. R. Landis and D. Umberson (1988), ‘Social relationships and health’, Science, 241, 540–45.

7. John T. Cacioppo, James H. Fowler and Nicholas A. Christakis (2009), ‘Alone in the crowd: The structure and spread of loneliness in a large social network’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 977–91.

8. Charles Darwin (1872), The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, London: John Murray.

9. J. M. Susskind et al. (2008), ‘Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition’, Nature Neuroscience, 11, 843–50.

10. V. Gallese, L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi and G. Rizzolatti (1996), ‘Action recognition in the premotor cortex’, Brain, 119, 593–609.

11. C. Keysers (2011), The Empathic Brain, Los Gatos, CA: Smashwords ebook.

12. This claim was made by the eminent neuroscientist Vilayanur Ramachandran and is related in C. Keysers (2011), The Empathic Brain, Los Gatos, CA: Smashwords ebook.

13. R. Mukamel, A. D. Ekstrom, J. Kaplan, M. Iacoboni and I. Fried (2010), ‘Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions’, Current Biology, 20, 750–56.

14. Cecilia Heyes (2010), ‘Where do mirror neurons come from?’, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 575–83.

15. Neha Mahajan & Karen Wynn (2012), ‘Origins of “Us” versus “Them”: Prelinguistic infants prefer similar others’, Cognition, 124, 227–33.

16. Gordon Gallup (1970), ‘Chimpanzees: Self-recognition’, Science, 167, 86–7. One curious exception is the domesticated dog, for reasons that are unknown. Apparently, domestication by man to be sociable did not include this component of social awareness.

17. B. Amsterdam (1972), ‘Mirror self-image reactions before age two’, Developmental Psychobiology, 5, 297–305.

18. Charles Darwin (1872), The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray, p. 325.

19. Mark R. Leary, Thomas W. Britt, William D Cutlip and Janice L. Templeton (1992), ‘Social blushing’, Psychological Bulletin, 112, 446–60.

20. K. M. Zosuls, D. Ruble, C. S. Tamis-LeMonda, P. E. Shrout, M. H. Bornstein and F. K. Greulich (2009), ‘The acquisition of gender labels in infancy: Implications for sex-typed play’, Developmental Psychology, 45, 688–701.

21. P. C. Quinn, J. Yahr, A. Kuhn, A. M. Slater and O. Pascalis (2002), ‘Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: A preference for female’, Perception 31, 1109–21.

22. E. E. Maccoby and C. N. Jacklin (1987), ‘Gender segregation in childhood’ in E. H. Reese (ed.), Advances in child development and behavior, vol. 20, New York: Academic Press, pp. 239–87.

23. H. Abel and R. Sahinkaya (1962), ‘Emergence of sex and race friendship preferences’, Child Development, 33, 939–43.

24. C. F. Miller, C. L. Martin, R. A. Fabes and L. D. Hanish (2013), ‘Bringing the Cognitive and Social Together’ in M. R. Banaji and S. A. Gelman (eds), Navigating the Social World. What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us, New York: Oxford University Press.

25. R. S. Bigler, C. S. Brown and M. Markell, M. (2001), ‘When groups are not created equal: Effects of group status on the formation of intergroup attitudes in children’, Child Development, 72, 1151–62.

26. R. S. Bigler (2013) ‘Understanding and Reducing Social Stereotyping and Prejudice Among Children’ in M. R. Banaji and S. A. Gelman (eds), Navigating the Social World. What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us, New York: Oxford University Press, p. 328.

27. Adam Waytz & Jason P. Mitchell (2011), ‘Two mechanisms for simulating other minds: Dissociations between mirroring and self-projection’, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 197–200.

28. Xiaojing Xu, Xiangyu Zuo, Xiaoying Wang and Shihui Han (2009), ‘Do you feel my pain? Racial group membership modulates empathic neural responses’, Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 8525–9.

29. Adrianna C. Jenkins and Jason P. Mitchell (2011), ‘Medial prefrontal cortex subserves diverse forms of self-reflection’, Social Neuroscience, 6, 211–18.

30. Kyungmi Kim and Marcia K. Johnson (2012), ‘Extended self: medial prefrontal activity during transient association of self and objects’, Scan, 7, 199–207.

31. M. Stel, J. Blascovich, C. McCall, J. Mastop, R. B. Van Baaren and R. Vonk (2010), ‘Mimicking disliked others: Effects of a priori liking on the mimicry-liking link’, European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 867–80.

32. Xiaojing Xu, Xiangyu Zuo, Xiaoying Wang and Shihui Han (2009), ‘Do you feel my pain? Racial group membership modulates empathic neural responses’, Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 8525–9.

33. Stanley Milgram (1963), ‘Behavioral study of obedience’, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 371–8.

34. P. Zimbardo (2007), The Lucifer Effect: How Good People Turn Evil, London: Random House.

35. H. Tajfel, M. G. Billig, R. P. Bundy and C. Flament (1971), ‘Social categorization and intergroup behaviour’, European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 149–78.

36. Martin Niemöller, ‘Als die Nazis die Kommunisten holten …’, http://www.martin-niemoeller-stiftung.de/4/daszitat/a31

37. S. Alexander Haslam and Stephen D. Reicher (2012), ‘Contesting the “Nature” of Conformity: What Milgram and Zimbardo’s studies really show’, PLOS Biology, volume 10, issue 11, e1001426.

38. N. Mahajan, M. A. Martinez, N. L. Gutierrez, G. Diesendruck, M. R. Banaji and L. R. Santos (2011), ‘The evolution of intergroup bias: perceptions and attitudes in rhesus macaques’, Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 100, 387–405.

39. Kate Fox (2004), Watching the English: The Hidden Rules of English Behaviour, Hodder & Stoughton, London.

40. Solomon E. Asch (1956), ‘Studies of independence and conformity: A minority of one against a unanimous majority’, Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 70, 1–70.

41. R. Bond and P. Smith (1996), ‘Culture and conformity: A meta-analysis of studies using Asch’s (1952b, 1956) line judgement task’, Psychological Bulletin, 119, 111–37.

42. Jamil Zaki, Jessica Schirmer and Jason P. Mitchell (2011), ‘Social influence modulates the neural computation of value’, Psychological Science, 22, 894–900.

43. J. Cloutier, T. F. Heatherton, P. J. Whalen and W. M. Kelley (2008), ‘Are attractive people rewarding? Sex differences in the neural substrates of facial attractiveness’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 941–51.

44. R.B. Cialdini (2005), ‘Don’t throw away the towel: Use social influence research’, American Psychological Society, 18, 33–4.

45. Richard H. Thaler and Cass R. Sunstein (2009, updated edition), Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness, New York: Penguin.

46. Leon Festinger (1957), A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

47. Vincent van Veen, Marie K. Krug, Jonathan W. Schooler and Cameron S. Carter (2009), ‘Neural activity predicts attitude change in cognitive dissonance’, Nature Neuroscience, 12, 1469–75.

48. Ellen J. Langer (1978), ‘Rethinking the Role of Thought in Social Interaction’ in John H. Harvey, William J. Ickes and Robert F. Kidd (eds), New Directions in Attribution Research, vol. 2, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 35–58.

49. Quoted in Carol Tavris and Elliot Aronson (2007), Mistakes Were Made (but not by me): Why We Justify Foolish Beliefs, Bad Decisions and Hurtful Acts, Harcourt Inc.

50. A. G. Greenwald and M. R. Banaji (1995), ‘Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes’, Psychological Review, 102, 4–27.

51. Andreas Olsson, Jeffrey P. Ebert, Mahzarin R. Banaji and Elizabeth A. Phelps (2005), ‘The role of social groups in the persistence of learned fear’, Science, 309, 785–7.

52. Carlos David Navarrete, Andreas Olsson, Arnold K. Ho, Wendy Berry Mendes, Lotte Thomsen and James Sidanius (2009), ‘Fear extinction to an out-group face: The role of target gender’, Psychological Science, 20, 155–8.

53. L.F. Pendry (2008), ‘Social cognition’ in M. R. Hewstone, W. Stroebe and K. Jonas (eds), Introduction to Social Psychology (fourth edition), Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 67–87.

54. Daniel Kahneman (2011), Thinking Fast, Thinking Slow, Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

55. J. Correll, B. Park, C. M. Judd and B. Wittenbrink (2002), ‘The police officer’s dilemma: Using ethnicity to disambiguate potentially threatening individuals’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1314–29.

56. M. Snyder. and W. B. Swann, Jr. (1978), ‘Hypothesis testing processes in social interaction’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1202–12.

57. D. F. Halpern (2004), ‘A cognitive-process taxonomy for sex differences in cognitive abilities’, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 135–9.

58. ‘It’s official: women are actually better parkers than men’: http://www.ncp.co.uk/documents/pressrelease/ncp-parking-survey.pdf

59. S. J. Spencer, C. M. Steele and D. M. Quinn (1999), ‘Stereotype threat and women’s math performance’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28.

60. C. M. Steele and J. Aronson (1995), ‘Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.

61. B. M. Hood, N. L. Gjersoe, K. Donnelly, A. Byers and S. Itajkura (2011), ‘Moral contagion attitudes towards potential organ transplants in British and Japanese adults’, Journal of Culture and Cognition, 11, 269–86.

62. C. Dyer (1999), ‘English teenager given heart transplant against her will’, British Medical Journal, 319(7204): 209.

63. M. A. Sanner (2005), ‘Living with a stranger’s organ: Views of the public and transplant recipients’, Annals of Transplantation, 10, 9–12.

64. D. L. Medin and A. Ortony (1989), ‘Psychological Essentialism’ in S. Vosniadou and A. Ortony (eds), Similarity and Analogical Reasoning, Cambridge University Press.

65. Susan A. Gelman (2003), The Essential Child: Origins of Essentialism in Everyday Thought, Oxford University Press.

66. Susan A. Gelman and Henry M. Wellman (1991), ‘Insides and essences: early understandings of the non-obvious’, Cognition, 38, 213–44.

67. Susan A. Gelman and Ellen M. Markman (1986), ‘Categories and induction in young children’, Cognition, 23, 183–209.

68. Meredith Meyer, Susan A. Gelman and Sarah-Jane Leslie (submitted), ‘My heart made me do it: Children’s essentialist beliefs about heart transplants’.

69. N. Haslam, B. Bastian, P. Bain and Y. Kashima (2006), ‘Psychological essentialism, implicit theories, and intergroup relations’, Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 9, 63–76.

70. Gil Diesendruck (2013), ‘Essentialism: The development of a simple, but potentially dangerous, idea’ in M. R. Banaji and S. A. Gelman (eds), Navigating the Social World. What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us, New York: Oxford University Press.

71. N. I. Eisenberger, M. D. Lieberman and K. D. Williams (2003), ‘Does rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion’, Science, 302, 290–92.

72. Nicole Legate, Cody R. DeHaan, Netta Weinstein and Richard M. Ryan (2013), ‘Hurting you hurts me too: The psychological costs of complying with ostracism’, Psychological Science, 24, 583–8.

73. K. D. Williams and S. A. Nida, (2011), ‘Ostracism: consequences and coping’, Current Directions in Psychology, 20, 71–5.

74. Lowell Gaertner, Jonathan Iuzzini and Erin M. O’Mara (2008), ‘When rejection by one fosters aggression against many: Multiple-victim aggression as a consequence of social rejection and perceived groupness’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 958–70.

75. W. A. Warburton, K. D. Williams and D. R. Cairns (2006), ‘When ostracism leads to aggression: The moderating effects of control deprivation’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 213–20.

76. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, ‘Youth suicide’. http://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/pub/youth_suicide.html Accessed 22 October 2013.

77. A. Brunstein Klomek, F. Marrocco, M. Kleinman, I.S. Schonfeld and M.S. Gould (2007), ‘Bullying, depression, and suicidality in adolescents’, Journal of the American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 40–49. http://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/pub/youth_suicide.html

78. Marcel F. van der Wal, Cees A. M. de Wit, Remy A. Hirasing (2003), Psychosocial health among young victims and offenders of direct and indirect bullying,. Pediatrics, 111, 1312–17.

79. Julie A. Paquette and Marion K. Underwood (1999), ‘Gender differences in young adolescents’ experiences of peer victimization: social and physical aggression’, Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Vol. 45: Issue 2, Article 5.

80. M. Boulton (1997), ‘Teachers’ views on bullying: Definitions, attitudes and ability to cope’, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 223–33.

81. K. D. Williams (2009), ‘Ostracism: A temporal need-threat model’, in M. Zanna (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, New York, Academic Press, 279–314.


EPILOGUE

1. Richard E. Nisbett (2003), The Geography of Thought, Nicholas Brealey Publishing.

2. Jared Diamond (1999), Guns, Germs, and Steel: The Fates of Human Societies, W. W. Norton & Co.

3. eMarketer Report (2013), Worldwide social network users: 2013 forecast and comparative estimates, http://www.emarketer.com/Article/Social-Networking-Reaches-Nearly-One-Four-Around-World/1009976 Accessed October 2013.

4. Adam Thierer (2013), ‘Technopanics, threat inflation, and the danger of an information technology precautionary principle’, Minnesota Journal of Law, Science & Technology, 14, 309–86.

5. Susan Greenfield (2009), ID: The Quest for Identity in the 21st Century: The Quest for Meaning in the 21st Century, Sceptre.

6. Phil Zimbardo (2012), The Demise of Guys: Why Boys Are Struggling and What We Can Do About It, TED publishing.

7. Independent Parliamentary Inquiry into Online Child Protection, April 2012.

8. http://www.theguardian.com/world/2010/mar/05/korean-girl-starved-online-game

9. Diana I. Tamir and Jason P. Mitchell (2012), ‘Disclosing information about the self is intrinsically rewarding’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 8038–804.

10. M. Naaman, J. Boase and C. H. Lai (2010), ‘Is it really about me?: Message content in social awareness streams’, Proceedings of the 2010 ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work (Association for Computing Machinery), Savannah, GA, pp. 189–92.

11. Leif Denti et al. (2012), ‘Sweden’s Largest Facebook Study: GRI rapport 2012–3’, https://gupea.ub.gu.se/bitstream/2077/28893/1/gupea_2077_28893_1.pdf

12. Amanda L. Forest & Joanne V. Wood (2012), ‘When social networking is not working: individuals with low self-esteem recognize but do not reap the benefits of self-disclosure on Facebook’, Psychological Science, 23, 295–302.

13. Robert B. Cialdini, Richard J. Borden, Avril Thorne, Marcus Randall Walker, Stephen Freeman and Lloyd Reynolds Sloan (1976), ‘Basking in reflected glory: Three (football) field studies’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 366–75.

14. Samsung poll and press release: http://www.samsung.com/uk/news/localnews/2013/samsung-nx300-wi-fi-every-day-over-1-million-photos-are-shot-and-shared-in-60-seconds

15. http://techcrunch.com/2012/08/22/how-big-is-facebooks-data-2–5-billion-pieces-of-content-and-500-terabytes-ingested-every-day/

16. M. D. Conover, J. Ratkiewicz, M. Francisco, B. Gonçalves, A. Flammini, and F. Menczer, ‘Political polarization on Twitter’, Proceedings of International Conference on Weblogs and Social Media 2011 (Unpublished, 2011), http://truthy.indiana.edu/site_media/pdfs/conover_icwsm2011_polarization.pdf

17. Sameer Hinduja and Justin W. Patchin (2010), ‘Bullying, cyberbullying, and suicide’, Archives of Suicide Research, 14, 206–21.

18. Vint Cerf speaking at the Consumer Electronic Show in January 2013: http://mashable.com/2013/01/09/would-you-wear-internet-connected-clothing/

19. Eli Pariser (2011), The Filter Bubble: What the Internet is Hiding From You, London: Penguin.

Загрузка...